In order to motivate to change a certain behavior, persuasive messages are among the effective ways. These messages are tailored to make an awareness of a problem, either framed in terms of intrinsic goals contrary to extrinsic goals, could make them more effective by increasing the level of self-motivation, targeting young drivers (Pelletier, 2008).
Many drivers use cell phones in their vehicles, which distracts. Hence one of the major traffic safety issues. In 2010, the NHTSA of United States conducted a telephone survey of approximately 6,001 drivers aged 18 years and above in all 50 States. The aim of this national survey was to examine recent attitudes and reported behaviors about distraction during driving (Braitman, 2010). While most drivers pick a call while driving, others with an equivalent number will continue driving after answering. Statistically, about 18% send text messages and e-mails while driving. An estimated 49% of those aged 21 to 24 years old admit to send text messages while driving, with an assumption that by doing so, the driving performance is not affected. Otherwise, 90% of passengers confirmed that they would feel not secure if their driver was using a cell phone. In the survey, most males and younger participants underestimated the negative impact of the cell phone on driving.
Generally, most drivers agree that driving is more dangerous when they fail to concentrate for 2 seconds or more. About 35% of drivers aged 18 to 24 years old reported that they can text at an average of 3 or more securely (Tison, 2011).
Adolescent drivers cause accidents majorly due to dangerous distraction by cell phones while driving. The NYDS, (National Young Driver Survey), carried out a research of (N = 5665) adolescent drivers to determine how psychosocial correlates with cell phone use on driving. The outcome showed more advantages on abstaining from using phones while driving (Hafetz, 2010).
High gain messages are framed as an investment to intervene to change of road user behavior. This becoming most effective techniques (Fylan, 2014).
On road use safety, the impact of the gain-framed or loss-framed messages on changing drivers’ intention has not been fully researched on. For the effective use of such messages, I hypothesize that; Gain-framed, high issue involvement inducing safety messages, or both about mobile phone use while driving will be more effective in changing people’s intentions towards this behavior.
Participants
The research participants were recruited from Macquarie University by convenience sampling. At the beginning, 892 responded to participate. Later, the number reduced to 343 after removing participants who didn’t read the entire involvement statement. That is, (less than ten seconds spent on screen) and those aged above 26 years. From this final data, the participants were 74% female and 26% males with an age range of 18 to 26 years old.
Materials
One of the materials used for this research was the modified intention measure obtained from Hatfield, Kasparian, Murphy and Job, 2005. Which was used to measure the intention of drivers about driving while fatigued, in this context, driving while responding to calls and texts (Hatfield, 2005).
In order to create a table showing the intention in driving while using a cell phone variable, results from different questions were averaged to create an intention score for analysis. In calculating the average, the pre-intention scores was negated from the post-intention score.
Procedures
After recruitment, the participants completed the study through the online survey platform Qualtrics provided. This was followed by filling of a demographic forms by every successful participant. The demographic questions consisted of the gender, age, ethnicity, education status or work.
Lastly, the participants filled the response forms related to the study for post measure and the pre measure of the intentions about using mobile phones while driving in the order of; high involvement/gain framed message, low involvement/loss framed message, high involvement/loss famed message and low involvement/gain famed message.
Descriptive statistics
Demographics
From the demographics, the mean age is 19.207 for N=343, the males 25.07%, females 74.34% and 0.58% others. The mean of years licensed for the participants was 3.018421.
Tabulation of mobile phone use.
How often do you use your phone while driving? |
Freq. |
percent |
Cum. |
Three or more times a week |
47 |
13.70 |
13.70 |
Once or twice a week |
93 |
27.11 |
40.82 |
Once or twice a month |
35 |
10.20 |
51.02 |
Less than once a week |
61 |
1778 |
68.80 |
Never |
107 |
3120 |
100 |
Total |
343 |
100 |
Tabulation of others mobile phone use
How often do you think others use their phone while driving? |
Freq. |
percent |
Cum. |
Three or more times a week |
194 |
56.56 |
56.56 |
Once or twice a week |
118 |
34.40 |
90.96 |
Once or twice a month |
21 |
6.12 |
97.08 |
Less than once a month |
6 |
1.75 |
98.83 |
Never |
4 |
1.17 |
100 |
Total |
343 |
100 |
The data of a measure of drivers’ intentions to talk on phone as well as their intentions to text while driving were collected and tabulated as shown below.
Hypothesis testing
Talking
Two-sample t test with equal variances
Variable |
Obs. |
Mean |
Std. err. |
Std. dev. |
95% conf. |
Loss Gain |
169 174 |
6.843195 5.675287 |
.9029805 1.000895 |
11.73875 13.20271 |
5.0605448.625846 3.69975 7.650825 |
Combined |
343 |
6.250729 |
.6748446 |
12.4983 |
4.92336 7.578097 |
Difference |
1.167908 |
1.35033 |
-1.488117 3.823933 |
Difference = mean(Loss) – mean(Gain) t = 0.8649
Ho: diff = 0 degrees of freedom = 341
Ha: diff < 0 Ha: diff!= 0 Ha: diff > 0
Pr(T < t) = 0.8062 Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.3877 Pr(T > t) = 0.1938
Hypothesis 2: High-issue involvement vs Low Issue involvement conditions
Two-sample t test with equal variances
variable |
Obs. |
Mean |
Std. err. |
Std. dev. |
95% conf. int |
Low |
182 |
4.431319 |
.845987 |
13.3611 |
2.762053 6.100584 |
High |
161 |
8.307453 |
1.053002 |
13.3611 |
6.227878 10.38703 |
Combined |
343 |
6.250729 |
.6748446 |
12.4983 |
7.578097 |
Difference |
-3.876135 |
1.337841 |
-6.507594 -1.244676 |
Difference= mean (Low) – mean (High) t = -2.8973
Ho: diff = 0 degrees of freedom = 341
Ha: diff < 0 Ha: diff! = 0 Ha: diff > 0
Pr(T < t) = 0.0020 Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.0040 Pr(T > t) = 0.9980
Hypothesis 3: Gain-framed High-issue involvement vs. Loss-framed High-issue involvement
Two-sample t test with equal variances
variable |
Obs. |
Mean |
Std. err. |
Std. dev. |
95% conf. |
Loss Gain |
81 80 |
8.37037 8.24375 |
1.399095 1.585037 |
12.59186 14.177 |
5.58608211.15466 5.08881311.39869 |
Combined |
161 |
8.307453 |
1.053002 |
13.3611 |
10.38703 |
Difference |
.1266204 |
2.112633 |
-4.045822 4.299063 |
Difference= mean (Loss) – mean (Gain) t = 0.0599
Ho: diff = 0 degrees of freedom = 159
Ha: diff < 0 Ha: diff! = 0 Ha: diff > 0
Pr(T < t) = 0.5239 Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.9523 Pr(T > t) = 0.4761
The graph of mean change in intention to talk on the phone while driving between the four groups.
Texting
Two-sample t test with equal variances
Variable |
Obs. |
Mean |
Std. err. |
Std. dev. |
95% conf. |
Loss Gain |
169 174 |
4.955621 4.563218 |
1.002022 1.046107 |
13.02628 13.7991 |
2.9774456.933798 2.498443 6.627994 |
Combined |
343 |
4.75656 |
.7238406 |
13.40572 |
3.33282 6.1803 |
Difference |
.3924029 |
1.449801 |
-2.459276 3.244081 |
Difference = mean (Loss) – mean (Gain) t = 0.2707
Ho: diff = 0 degrees of freedom = 341
Ha: diff < 0 Ha: diff != 0 Ha: diff > 0
Pr(T < t) = 0.6066 Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.7868 Pr(T > t) = 0.3934
Hypothesis 2: High-issue involvement vs Low Issue involvement conditions
Two-sample t test with equal variances
Variable |
Obs. |
Mean |
Std. err. |
Std. dev. |
95% conf. interval |
Low |
182 |
4.568681 |
1.008786 |
2.578188 |
6.559174 |
High |
161 |
4.968944 |
1.041172 |
2.912732 |
7.025156 |
Combined |
343 |
4.75656 |
.7238406 |
13.40572 |
3.33282 6.1803 |
Difference |
-.4002628 |
1.452366 |
-3.256986 |
2.456461 |
Difference = mean (Low) – mean (High) t = -0.2756
Ho: diff = 0 degrees of freedom = 341
Ha: diff < 0 Ha: diff != 0 Ha: diff > 0
Pr(T < t) = 0.3915 Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.7830 Pr(T > t) = 0.6085
Hypothesis 3: Gain-framed High-issue involvement vs. Loss-framed High-issue involvement
Two-sample t test with equal variances
Variable |
Obs. |
Mean |
Std. err. |
Std. dev. |
95% conf. interval |
Loss |
81 |
4.895062 |
1.259952 |
11.33956 |
7.402445 |
Gain |
80 |
5.04375 |
1.670533 |
14.9417 |
8.368862 |
Combined |
161 |
4.968944 |
1.041172 |
13.21099 |
7.025156 |
Difference |
-.1486883 |
2.088889 |
3.976859 |
Difference = mean (Loss) – mean (Gain) t = -0.0712
Ho: diff = 0 degrees of freedom = 159
Ha: diff < 0 Ha: diff!= 0 Ha: diff > 0
Pr(T < t) = 0.4717 Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.9433 Pr(T > t) = 0.528
Graph representing the mean change in intention to text while driving between the four groups.
The study was set to investigate the whether the message framing in terms of a gain or a loss impact to change drivers’ intentions and how the effectiveness of such messages can be increased by introducing high issue involvement to the individuals (Chaurand, 2015). From the research analysis, all the hypothesis are supported. Evidently, the high gain framed together with high involvement is effective way to change the intentions of the driver as it is a psychological factor. This can effectively reduce accidents among young drivers (Cazzulino, 2014).
In conclusion, the road safety campaign is dependent on its effectiveness to change the intention of drivers. The high gain frame messages with high involvement can be adopted as one of the current measures (Hoekstra, 2011). This practice for road safety has a psychological influence especially in change the intention of individual on the subject matter and this can help promote safe driving among young people (Millar, 2000).
References
Braitman, K. A. (2010). National reported patterns of driver cell phone use in the United States. . Traffic injury prevention, 11(6), 543-548.
Cazzulino, F. B. (2014). Cell phones and young drivers: a systematic review regarding the association between psychological factors and prevention . Traffic injury prevention, 15(3), 234-242.
Chaurand, N. B. (2015). A naturalistic study of the impact of message framing on highway speeding. Transportation research part F. traffic psychology and behaviour, 35, 37-44.
Fylan, F. &. (2014). Behavioural Change Techniques used in road safety interventions for young people. . Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée/European Review of Applied Psychology, 64(3), 123-129.
Hafetz, J. S.-E. (2010). Adolescent drivers’ perceptions of the advantages and disadvantages of abstention from in-vehicle cell phone use. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(6), 1570-157.
Hatfield, J. M. (2005). Risk perceptions, attitudes and behaviours regarding driver fatigue in NSW youth: The Development of an Evidence-Based Driver Fatigue Educational Intervention Strategy. Report to the Motor Accidents Authority of NSW.
Hoekstra, T. &. (2011). Improving the effectiveness of road safety campaigns: Current and new practices. . IATSS Research, 34, 80-86.
Millar, M. G. (2000). Promoting safe driving behaviors: The influence of message framing and issue involvement. Journal of applied social psychology, 30(4), 853-866.
Pelletier, L. G. (2008). Persuasive communication and proenvironmental behaviours: How message tailoring and message framing can improve the integration of behaviours through self-determined motivation. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne 49(3), 210.
Tison, J. C. (2011, 12). TRB: National phone survey on distracted driving attitudes and behaviors. Retrieved from TRID: https://rosap.ntl.bts.gov/view/dot/192
Essay Writing Service Features
Our Experience
No matter how complex your assignment is, we can find the right professional for your specific task. Contact Essay is an essay writing company that hires only the smartest minds to help you with your projects. Our expertise allows us to provide students with high-quality academic writing, editing & proofreading services.Free Features
Free revision policy
$10Free bibliography & reference
$8Free title page
$8Free formatting
$8How Our Essay Writing Service Works
First, you will need to complete an order form. It's not difficult but, in case there is anything you find not to be clear, you may always call us so that we can guide you through it. On the order form, you will need to include some basic information concerning your order: subject, topic, number of pages, etc. We also encourage our clients to upload any relevant information or sources that will help.
Complete the order formOnce we have all the information and instructions that we need, we select the most suitable writer for your assignment. While everything seems to be clear, the writer, who has complete knowledge of the subject, may need clarification from you. It is at that point that you would receive a call or email from us.
Writer’s assignmentAs soon as the writer has finished, it will be delivered both to the website and to your email address so that you will not miss it. If your deadline is close at hand, we will place a call to you to make sure that you receive the paper on time.
Completing the order and download